Novel Object Recognition Test |Paddling Pool Task |Barnes Maze |Spontaneous Alternation Mazes (T and Y mazes) |Morris Water Maze |Porsolt Forced Swim Test |Tail Suspension Test  |Use of Electric Shock in Animals |Stereotypic Behavior in Rodents

Rodent Behavior and Behavioral Testing

Many different standardized behavior tests exist in rodent research. For best results, investigators should familiarize themselves with the intent and methodology of a test before committing to using it in their reseach protocols. Behavioral tests must be adequately described and justified in your IACUC protocol prior to approval for use. 

A short description and basic guidelines for some commonly used behavioral tests are included on this page. PI's may use this information to assist them in completing their IACUC protocol and for guidance in planning their experimental procedures.

Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Test 

A type of spontaneous preference test in which recognition memory (i.e., ability to distinguish novel from familiar stimuli) is inferred by the greater lengths of time spent exploring novel versus familiar objects in an open field or maze4. This test takes advantage of normal, spontaneous exploratory behavior and has been used in many species including rats and mice. However, in rodents the context in which a novel object is encountered affects the level of exploration. In rats and mice, a novel object in an unfamiliar environment (e.g., open field) is readily explored but a novel object in a familiar environment (i.e., home cage) may be avoided and buried1.

Various forms of the NOR test are used to assess short- and long-term memory, memory consolidation/reconsolidation, spatial and episodic memory, pattern separation and cross-modal recognition3. Advantages of the NOR include the spontaneous nature of animal responses (i.e., no requirement for food deprivation or other aversive stimuli or appetitive rewards that may influence results); no required training other than habituation to the test arena; task difficulty can be varied by changing the interval between habituation and test sessions; and testing can be completed in few sessions. Disadvantages include reliance on locomotor activity for test accuracy; individual/strain variability in behavior and preference for specific types of objects (requires counterbalance of novel and familiar object choice); and a lack of standardization of pre-test habituation procedures and objects used as test stimuli. In addition, running the test in an open field requires repeated animal handling which increases animal stress. A less stressful alternative may be to run the test in a bow-tie maze in which multiple trials within one session are completed without handling in between4.

Commonly used variations of the NOR include2,3:

  • Novel object recognition (NOR) – Test animals must recognize a novel object (what). Primarily evaluates perirhinal cortex function.
  • Object location (OL) – Test animals must recognize a familiar object in a novel spatial location (where). Primarily evaluates hippocampal function.
  • Temporal order recognition (TOR) – Test animals are exposed to objects they have interacted with at different points in time (when) and must recognize the least familiar object. Primarily evaluates connections between hippocampus, perirhinal and prefrontal cortex.

 

Troubleshooting:

Tests may be run in an open field or a maze (e.g., bow tie maze4). Large differences in test object size may result in recognition performance differences when using mazes3.

Features such as shape, size, color, brightness, texture and odor can determine how an animal recognizes a test object. There is limited knowledge of which physical features rats and mice use when discriminating between test objects; however, recognition memory performance often varies inversely with similarity (i.e., increasing number of shared features) between test objects3.

Exploration parameters should be of test object exploration not overall exploration of the open field [e.g., snout contact or orientation toward object within a specific distance from the object, crossing a line surrounding the location of an object or head placement within a specific distance from the perimeter of an object (zone entries)]. However, zone entries (often used by automated video tracking systems) may not be an accurate measure of object exploration and can fail to reveal discrimination between novel and familiar objects3.

The number of pre-test habituation sessions used varies but Ennaceur3 recommends five (1/day/10 minutes each) to decrease emotional reactivity to the open field or maze. However, rodent strains vary in emotional reactivity to novelty, so the number of habituation sessions may be influenced by strain (e.g., C57BL/6J and CD-1 = 1-2 sessions; BALB/c = 4-5 sessions; Wistar rats = 4 sessions3). Pre-test habituation sessions can also be used to evaluate spontaneous locomotor and exploratory activity3.

References:

  1. Blaser R, Heyser C. Spontaneous object recognition: a promising approach to the comparative study of memory. 2015. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9: 183.
  2. Cruz-Sanchez, A., Dematagoda, S., Ahmed, R. et al. Developmental onset distinguishes three types of spontaneous recognition memory in mice. 2020. Sci Rep 10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67619-w
  3. Ennaceur A. Object Novelty Recognition Memory Test. Ed: Ennaceur A, de Souza Silva MA, Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. 2018. 27, 1-22. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-812012-5.00001-X.
  4. Kinnavane L, Albasser MM, Aggleton JP.  Advances in the behavioural testing and network imaging of rodent recognition memory. 2015. Behavioural Brain Research 285, 67-78.

 

Paddling Pool Task

Synonyms: Oxford Paddling Pool test

The paddling pool task (PPT) has been shown to be a less averse, mouse specific spatial cognition test that combines aspects of the Morris water and Barnes mazes. This test is used to assess hippocampus-based learning in mice. The PPT, compared to the Morris water maze, minimizes anxiety, exhaustion and hypothermia which are known to be significant interfering factors in mouse performance in the Morris water maze.

In the PPT the test mouse is placed in a circular tank filled with cold water (i.e., 18° +/- 1°C) to a depth of 2 cm. The tank has 12 escape holes located around the periphery (‘clockmaze’). One hole is open (true exit) to a dry escape corridor and the other 11 holes are blocked. Specific objects (visual cues) are placed around the outside of the tank. The mouse can use the visual cues to find the true exit more quickly each time it completes a trial. Mice undergo a period of pre-training to gain familiarity with the apparatus. For the test itself, the mouse is given a series of learning trials in the tank in which they can paddle (walking in the water) until they find the true exit. Each learning trial lasts a specific amount of time and the time between trials must also be specified. Parameters measured during the learning trials include escape latency and number of errors (blocked exits visited). Following this, a “probe trial” may be run in which all escape holes are blocked and the time the animal spends near the previously open exit is measured. Animals that have learned the position of the true exit will spend most of their time in the area where it was located. Animals that are poor learners will search other areas of the tank.

See this link for photos of the PPT.

Species used: This test was developed for mice. Special consideration must be given to the use of this test in mouse strains or genotypes with reduced ability to navigate using spatial cues, e.g., visual impairments.

Important considerations:

As with all behavioral tests, transportation of animals and set up of the experimental area must be carefully planned to limit exposure of test mice to unnecessary light, vibrations, noise, or other stressful events that can influence behavior. Behavioral tests are frequently done during the dark phase of the light cycle and mice should not be exposed to bright light prior to the testing period (e.g., during transport to the testing room). Animals benefit from prior acclimatization to handling and pre-test training to familiarize the mice to the apparatus is important.

  1. The tank water for the PPT must be cold enough to stimulate the mice to actively explore the tank. Published temperatures range from 18-21°C. Water temperature must be monitored continuously; ice cubes may be added to keep it cold. If water temperatures are too high, mice may remain immobile in the center of the pool.
  2. The water depth is usually 2 cm. Mice must be able to constantly touch the floor of the pool.
  3. Twelve escape holes are arranged equidistantly around the tank perimeter. The lower edge of each hole must be at mouse head level. Escape hole diameter is usually 40-50 mm. Mice may be reluctant to enter holes that are too large or small. During the test, eleven of the escape holes are sealed to prevent mouse entry. Hole plugs should be flush with the internal pool surface and look like the open escape hole. Plugs should be the same color (e.g., black) as the open escape hole which is attached to a black plastic pipe.
  4. Tap water is used to fill the PPT tank. The floor of the tank may be colored white to increase aversiveness and encourage escape.
  5. Urine and fecal material will accumulate in the water and contribute to bacterial contamination and growth. The tank should be drained and disinfected after each day's trials. Partial water changes between mice may reduce the accumulation of urine/fecal material. Fecal material may be removed after each animal with a small mesh net.
  6. After completing a trial, the mouse may be placed in a clean plastic cage under a heat lamp for a few minutes to dry. Towel or blow drying is not recommended.

 

Test Procedures

  1. Quickly release the mouse just above water level in the center of the pool. Slowly releasing the mouse may lead to struggling and can impair initial orientation.
  2. Maximum trial length is 60 seconds. The mouse is gently guided toward the exit if the trial time is exceeded.
  3. Inter-trial intervals are 15-20 minutes with up to 4 trials per day for 3-4 days.

 

References:

Deacon, R. M. J. Shallow Water (Paddling) Variants of Water Maze Tests in Mice. J. Vis. Exp. (76), e2608, doi:10.3791/2608 (2013).

Stankowski R, et al. Large Scale Validation of the Paddling Pool Task in the Clockmaze for Studying Hippocampus-Based Spatial Cognition in Mice. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 07 June 2019.

Barnes Maze

Mazes, such as the Morris water maze (MWM), Barnes maze and t maze, are commonly used to evaluate spatial learning and memory in laboratory rodents. These mazes assume that animals use visual-spatial signals to learn and remember a location that provides safety, food, water or some other reward. However, the complexity of animal behavior required to complete these tests means that other cognitive or non-cognitive processes cannot be excluded. In addition, the diversity of mazes used and the large number of variables that influence animal performance means that not all mazes evaluate the same cognitive process. Mice have been shown to use three different search strategies: random, spatial and serial, in the Barnes maze (Sunyer B et al.) The Barnes maze tests learning, memory and cognitive flexibility. It was developed for use in rats, then modified for mice. C57BL mouse strains have shown good learning and memory in the Barnes maze while other strains have shown poor performance.

The maze consists of a circular table with circular holes around the circumference. Visual cues placed by the experimenter (e.g., colored shapes) and as part of the room (e.g., door, furniture) are located near the maze. The rodent must search for and find a target box positioned beneath one of the holes to escape an aversive stimulus such as loud noise or bright light. Training is required as the rodent must learn the position of the target box before being tested.

Various published training protocols are available. All consist of the following phases:

  • Habituation - allows animals to learn that the target box is a rewarding place to be.
  • Acquisition - allows animals to learn how to find the target box as quickly as possible.
  • Probe trial - probe trial(s) are conducted 24 hours (short-term) and/or several days (long-term, e.g., 10-12 days) after the last training (acquisition) day to assess short- and long-term memory.

Advantages (versus MWM):

  1. Does not involve swimming so is less stressful than the MWM. In addition, there is no reduction in core body temperature and mice cannot float to avoid performing.
  2. Allow clear delineation of the three possible search strategies used by mice during each trial.

Potential problems:

  1. Lack of stressful stimuli in the Barnes maze may slow learning. White noise of loud buzzers can be used to increase stress (and motivation to find the target box).
  2. Requires more training than the MWM (~15-20 acquisition trials versus ~12 for MWM).
  3. Most published literature on Barnes maze in mice used C57BL/6 mice (3-5 months old). Rates of learning vary substantially between individuals so group size must be at least 10-12.
  4. Performance in both MWM and Barnes maze are highly sensitive to anxiety in rodents (e.g., pharmacological and genetic manipulations) although this appears to be more of a concern in the MWM. Anxiety may be reduced by lowering light intensity.

References:

Pitts MW. Barnes Maze Procedure for Spatial Learning and Memory in Mice. Bio Protoc 2018 Mar 5; 8(5): e2744. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5891830/

Sunyer B, Patil S, Hoger H et al. Barnes maze, a useful task to assess spatial reference memory in the mice. 2007 Oct 4, PROTOCOL (Version 1) available at Protocol Exchange https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2007.390

Spontaneous Alternation Mazes (T and Y mazes)

Information provided for this description taken from: d’Isa R, Comi G & Leocani L.  d'Isa R, Comi G, Leocani L. Apparatus design and behavioural testing protocol for the evaluation of spatial working memory in mice through the spontaneous alternation T-maze. Sci Rep. 2021 Oct 27;11(1):21177. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00402-7. PMID: 34707108; PMCID: PMC8551159.

Well established test of spatial working memory in rodents that, along with the object recognition test, is considered among the least stressful and animal friendly options for assessment of cognitive function. The T maze is a T-shaped apparatus that provides a choice between two opposite arms. Spontaneous alternation refers to the natural tendency of rodents to prefer exploring a novel maze arm over a familiar one, which leads them to alternate the choice of the goal arm in the T maze test. The animal must remember which arm was visited in the previous trial to correctly choose the less familiar one. The T maze is considered more sensitive in evaluating hippocampal function than the Morris water maze.

In mice, a series of test trials are conducted (~5-12) with an intertrial interval (ITI) of up to 60 seconds. No pre-test training is required. The maze is placed in one location (only) in a dimly lit testing room (to ensure consistency of extra-maze visual cues). Percentages of alternation across the test trials are calculated as an index of working memory. Normal wild-type mice typically show an index of 70-75%, well above chance level (50%). Spontaneous alternation rates are inversely proportional to ITI and task difficulty can be increased by increasing ITI. The T maze test may not be suitable for mice with severe motor impairments because they may not be able to complete test trials within the fixed cut-off time (before working memory fades).

Published variations of the basic T-maze protocol include:

Rewarded alternation: Correct alternations are reinforced by the experimenter using a food reward. This version of the t maze test does not appear to be as sensitive in detecting cognitive impairment as the spontaneous t maze and requires food deprivation (increases stress for the subject animals).

Continuous trials: The test is performed in a Y-maze (equal 120° angles between arms) and animals are allowed to freely explore the maze for a specific amount of time or number of arm visits. Correct alternation is scored when the animal chooses to enter one of the two arms not visited in the previous trial, or alternatively, when all three arms are explored consecutively. This test version avoids stress from handling in between trials but may not detect hippocampal dysfunction. In addition, ITI is uncontrollable in this test version. Fixed ITI is important because spatial working memory lasts for only a short time.

Enclosed arms (versus open): Less anxiety producing in rodents (may require less time to habituate) and prevents escape by jumping off arms.

Tips for success:

  • When possible, house mice in pairs or groups to lessen stress associated with individual housing. Published studies have shown that isolation stress in mice can result in increased anxiety, neophobia, aggression, and locomotor activity and impair short- and long-term memory, alter sensorimotor gating and decrease cognitive flexibility. However, male mice in strains with a high incidence of territorial aggression may need to be housed individually as repeated stress and injuries from fighting also affect welfare and behavior.
  • Mice are traditionally tested during the dark phase so housing under a reversed light cycle is recommended.
  • Environmental enrichment of home caging is encouraged to maximize animal welfare.
  • Do not change home cages on the day before testing (ensure at least 48 hours of habituation to a new cage prior to testing).
  • Do not handle mice by the tail for placement and retrieval in the maze as this is highly stressful. Tunnel handling or cupping within the hands is preferred (requires pre-test addition of tunnel in home cage or training to habituate to handling within cupped hands).

Troubleshooting:

  1. If test results indicate a side preference (left or right arm of maze), the experimental room environment and maze design/set up should be reviewed to eliminate unexpected stimuli such as non-uniform lighting or odor from nearby mice.
  2. Normal mice spontaneously alternate above chance level (between 70 and 80%). If control mice show chance level alteration check the following:
    • No auditory, visual or olfactory distractions in the experimental room during testing.
    • Verify that housing conditions are normal with no unexpected stressful events or situations.
    • Check that the home cage has not been changed within 48 hours of the test start.
    • Make sure mice have not undergone stressful manipulations in the previous days (e.g., anesthesia and surgery). At least five days of recovery are recommended before behavioral testing.
  3. Choice latencies should not be too high (i.e., group average T0 <30 s; T1-T6 mean <60 s). Latencies commonly increase over trials (T0 to T6) within one test session. In addition, repeating the entire test (i.e., multiple test sessions) will result in the exclusion of increasing numbers of mice because they will exceed the time limit for task completion. No more than three test replications should be used unless sample sizes are great enough to compensate for this loss. Gentle auditory or tactile stimulation may be used to encourage mice to finish a test trial if required but must be used consistently and carefully to avoid influencing results.

Reference:

d’Isa R, Comi G & Leocani L.  d'Isa R, Comi G, Leocani L. Apparatus design and behavioural testing protocol for the evaluation of spatial working memory in mice through the spontaneous alternation T-maze. Sci Rep. 2021 Oct 27;11(1):21177. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00402-7. PMID: 34707108; PMCID: PMC8551159.

Morris Water Maze

Synonyms: (submerged platform) water escape task, Morris water escape task, Morris water navigation task.

Mazes, such as the Morris water maze (MWM), Barnes maze and t maze, are commonly used to evaluate spatial learning and memory in laboratory rodents. These mazes assume that animals use visual-spatial signals to learn and remember a location that provides safety, food , water or some other reward. However, the complexity of animal behavior required to complete these tests means that other cognitive or non-cognitive processes cannot be excluded. In addition, the diversity of mazes used and the large number of variables that influence animal performance means that not all mazes evaluate the same cognitive process. 

In the MWM the animal is required to swim in a tank of opaque water until it finds a submerged platform that it can mount to escape the water. Presumably, the animal uses specific visual cues placed around the outside of the tank to learn where the platform is located. An animal that is able to remember the cues will find the platform more quickly each time it completes a trial swim. Animals are initially given a series of “learning trials” in which they are allowed to swim in the tank until they find the platform. Each learning trial lasts a specific amount of time and the time between trials must also be specified. Following this, a “probe trial” is run in which the submerged platform is removed and the time the animal spends swimming in the quadrant of the tank where the platform was previously located is measured. Animals that have learned the position of the platform will spend most of their time in the quadrant where the platform was previously located. Animals that are poor learners will spend time searching other areas of the tank.

See this link for an illustration1 of the Morris water maze.

Species used:  Rats and mice. This task was developed for use in rats (generally good swimmers). In mice, performance in this test is highly dependent on genetic background and other variables. Special consideration must be given to the use of this test in mouse strains or genotypes with reduced ability to navigate using spatial cues or to swim, e.g., visual or musculoskeletal impairments. In addition, mice with other physiological or behavioral traits such as impaired thermogenesis or high anxiety levels may also perform poorly in this test1.

Important considerations:

Water Tank

  1. Size (diameter) and depth of the tank (varies with species). Water depth of 15-20 cm is adequate for mice. Rats are larger and may dive to the bottom so require deeper water.
  2. Size of platform in relation to the diameter of the tank (task difficulty increases with decreasing size of the platform).
  3. A round shape is recommended for the escape platform (provides the same tactile cues on all sides)3. The platform surface should be textured so the animal can maintain a secure grip and close enough to the water surface so the majority of the animal’s body is out of the water when on top of the platform (e.g., mice ≤0.5 cm below water surface).
  4. Mice are more susceptible to hypothermia than rats. Hypothermia risk can be lessened by increasing the water temperature and/or increasing the inter-trial interval. Water temperatures < 20⁰C can lead to hypothermia. Temperatures that are too warm may discourage active swimming/searching. Animals should be allowed to dry in a warm environment after removal from the tank. Absorbent towel(s) may be placed in the holding cage to collect water dripping off the animal and a heating source directed over or underneath the cage may provide warmth. Do not attempt to towel- or blow -dry animals as this is stressful and rough handling can cause injury.
  5. Substances used to make the water opaque must be edible and nontoxic because animals will consume the substance when grooming after each trial. Tempera paint is recommended3. Milk supports bacterial growth, especially in warm water and lime or chalk may be toxic. Alternatives that may allow the use of clear water include using a clear Plexiglas platform or using a platform the same color as the tank surface.
  6. Cleaning schedule for the tank (water changes). Urine and fecal material will accumulate in the water and contribute to bacterial contamination and growth. The tank should be drained and disinfected after each day’s trials. Partial water changes between mice can reduce the accumulation of urine/fecal material. Fecal material may be removed after each animal with a small mesh net3.

 

Test Procedures 

  1. Trials should not exceed 2 minutes.
  2. Inter-trial intervals should be long enough to prevent the development of hypothermia and fatigue over repeated trials. It is recommended that intervals be at least 10-15 minutes, especially in mice5.
  3. Animals must be observed continuously while in the tank and removed from the water if their head sinks below the surface4.
  4. Two to four trials per day are generally adequate for training.

 

Alternative tests for spatial learning and memoryPaddling Pool Task, Radial arm maze, Barnes circular platform maze

References: 

  1. Terry AV Jr. Spatial Navigation (Water Maze) Tasks. In: Buccafusco JJ, editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2nd edition. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press; 2009. Chapter 13. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK5217/
  2. Crawley JN. What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice, 2nd ed. Hoboken (NJ): Wiley; 2007.
  3. Wahlsten D. Mouse Behavioral Testing: How to use Mice in Behavioral Neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2011.
  4. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2003.
  5. Iivonen H, Nurminen L, Harri M, Tanila H, Puolivali J (2003). Hypothermia in mice tested in Morris Water Maze. Behav Brain Res 141: 207-213.

 

Porsolt Forced Swim Test

Purpose: The Porsolt swim test (PST) was developed as a rodent screening test for potential (human) antidepressant drugs. It is based on the assumption that an animal will try to escape an aversive (stressful) stimulus. If escape is impossible, the animal eventually stops trying and gives up. In the PST, the animal is placed in a cylindrical container of water from which it cannot escape. Most animals will attempt to escape by actively swimming. When the animal stops swimming and floats on the surface of the water it is considered to have “given up”.  An animal that gives up relatively quickly is thought to be displaying characteristics similar to human depression.  The validity of this test stems from the finding that physical or psychological stress (which can induce depression in humans) administered prior to the test causes animals to give up sooner and treatment with an antidepressant drug will increase the time an animal spends in escape attempts.

Species used: Rats and mice. Impaired swimming ability due to musculoskeletal or other abnormalities will affect performance in this test.

Important considerations

  1. The water must be deep enough so the animal cannot touch the bottom with its tail or feet. A depth of 30 cm is commonly recommended, although less depth may be adequate for mice. Water temperature should be 24-30⁰C2.
  2. Animals should be allowed to dry in a warm environment after removal from the water. Absorbent towel(s) may be placed in the holding cage to collect water dripping off the animal and a heating source directed over or underneath the cage may provide warmth. Do not attempt to towel- or blow-dry animals as this is stressful and rough handling can cause injury.
  3. Water changes:  Urine and fecal material will accumulate in the water and contribute to bacterial contamination and growth. The container should be emptied and disinfected after each day’s tests. Fecal material may be removed after each animal with a small mesh net.
  4. Test procedures:  A wide range of test session durations have been reported (4-20 minutes)1Animals must be observed continuously during the swim test. Any animal that sinks below the surface should be removed from the water immediately2.

 

Alternative tests: Tail-suspension test and others1,2,3.

References: 

  1. Crawley JN. What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice, 2nd ed. Hoboken (NJ): Wiley; 2007.
  2. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2003. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK43327/
  3. Castagne V, Moser P, Porsolt RD. Behavioral Assessment of Antidepressant Activity in Rodents. In: Buccafusco JJ, editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2nd edition. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press, 2009. Chapter 6. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK5222/

 

Tail Suspension Test

Purpose: The tail suspension test (TST) was developed as a rodent screening test for potential (human) antidepressant drugs. It is based on the assumption that an animal will actively try to escape an aversive (stressful) stimulus. If escape is impossible, the animal will eventually stop trying ("give up"). In the TST a mouse is suspended by the tail so that its body dangles in the air, facing downward. The test lasts for six or more minutes and may be repeated multiple times. Mice initially struggle to face upward and climb to a solid surface. When the animal stops struggling and hangs immobile it is considered to have “given up”.  Longer periods of immobility are characteristic of a depressive-like state.  The validity of this test stems from the finding that treatment with an antidepressant drug will decrease the time the animal spends immobile.

Species used: mice

Important Considerations:

  1. Mice are suspended (a variable distance) above a solid surface by the use of adhesive tape applied to the tail. If the tape is incorrectly applied or fails, the mouse will fall. The use of a “cushioned” surface below the TST may be needed to help prevent injury to the animal. Mice that experience a fall should be removed from the experiment1.
  2. Vinyl or medical adhesive tape is recommended. Duct tape is too adhesive and will tear hair and skin when removed1. The tape should be applied in a consistent position ¾ of the distance from the base of the mouse’s tail2. If the tape is applied too near the tip of the tail it may pull off the skin of the tail tip and the mouse will fall.
  3. Some strains (e.g., C57BL/6J) may not perform well in the TST due to tail climbing behavior. Strains with vestibular deficits may show an abnormal spinning phenotype and should not be used in the TST. Other mouse phenotypes that display neurological abnormalities that lead to unusual leg clasping behavior or that influence immobility times may also not be appropriate models for this test1.

 

Alternative tests: Porsolt swim test and others2.

References:

  1. CL Bergner, AN Smolinsky, PC Hart, BD Dufour, RJ Egan, JL LaPorte, AV Kalueff. 2010. Mouse Models for Studying Depression-Like States and Antidepressant Drugs.  In: Mouse Models for Drug Discovery, Methods in Molecular Biology 602: 267-282.
  2. Castagné V, Moser P, Porsolt RD. Behavioral Assessment of Antidepressant Activity in Rodents. In: Buccafusco JJ, editor. Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2nd edition. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press; 2009. Chapter 6. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK5222/
  3. B Thierry, L Steru, P Simon, RD Porsolt. 1986. The tail suspension test: Ethical considerations. Psychopharmacology 90: 284-285.

 

Use of Electric Shock in Research Animals

Purpose: Electric shock is used as an aversive stimulus in behavioral testing with humans and other animals, including invertebrates. Aversive stimuli function as a type of negative reinforcement: The frequency of a measured behavior increases in order to end or avoid the aversive stimulus. Electric shock is favored as an aversive stimulus because it is easily quantifiable; can be manipulated to have discrete or gradual onset and offset; and (at levels typically used in research) does not cause physical damage to the subject. The disadvantage of electric shock includes the fact that it can be painful and is an “unnatural” stimulus (i.e., not normally experienced outside the laboratory). Electric shock stimulates uncontrolled muscle contractions and will result in (increasing) pain as intensity increases.

Species used: Many species although rodents are most commonly used. Impaired motor coordination due to musculoskeletal or other abnormalities will affect performance if animals are expected to coordinate movements to escape the electric shock.

Important considerations

Shock Intensity

  1. The level of shock intensity used must be sufficient to elicit a reaction in the animal but not enough to injure or create unnecessary pain or distress.
  2. The investigator must be familiar with the capacity of their equipment and the shock levels typically applied in the species under study. Devices designed for larger animals (e.g., rats) may not be suitable for mice.
  3. Some authors recommend that the shock intensity being used be evaluated daily by placing a hand onto the electric grid while the shock is being delivered. No more than a “mild tingling” should be felt1.
  4. Shock delivered in pulses provides for “shock-free intervals” that allow more effective escape attempts by the animal2.
  5. Water decreases the electrical resistance of skin and other tissues. The presence of urine or other sources of moisture will increase the shock intensity experienced by the animal.
  6. Electric current delivered to a small area of skin is perceived as more aversive than the same current applied to a larger area2. An animal standing on a rough surface may perceive greater shock intensity than one standing on a smooth surface.
  7. Species, genetic background and other intrinsic variables may influence an animal’s degree of sensitivity and type of response to shock and must be considered2. Please consult the references listed at the end of this section for additional information on how to design and set up experiments using electric shock (in rodents).

 

Test procedures

  1. Do not require animals to perform complex or skilled maneuvers to escape shock2.
  2. Mice show two primary reactions to electric shock: Jumping and running. Genotype will influence which reaction predominates in a strain. Investigators may want to consider the typical reaction pattern of the strain(s) they are using when planning what type of escape response will be required by the animal (e.g., a strain that responds to shock by running may have difficulty learning to escape if jumping is required to leave the shock chamber)2.
  3. If animals can retreat to non-electrified areas within the apparatus (e.g., chamber edges) they may be able to avoid the shock. This is more likely to occur when tasks are too difficult and cannot be learned quickly2.

 

Alternative types of aversive stimuli or methodology:  Air puffs, loud noises, bright lights or ultrasonic tones. Alternative training methods include the use of a reward (e.g., preferred food) for correct responses instead of punishment (electric shock) for incorrect responses.

For more information on test procedures and experimental design please consult the following references: 

  1. Graham JH and Buccafusco JJ (2001). Inhibitory Avoidance Behavior and Memory Assessment. In Buccafusco JJ (ed.), Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience, p.141-151. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
  2. Wahlsten D (2011). Mouse Behavioral Testing: How to Use Mice in Behavioral Neuroscience. London: Academic Press.

 

Social, Maternal and Aggressive Behaviors in Rodents

Many standard behavioral tests exist for the study of interactive behavior in mice and rats. In order to choose the most appropriate test for a research study it is important to understand something about the range of rodent social behaviors and what, specifically, behavioral tests are attempting to measure. Rodent social behavior may be classified into general categories such as aggression and social dominance behavior; parental and maternal behavior; and social recognition and approach behavior. Specific tests are designed to investigate behavioral differences in each of these categories.

Rats and mice used in research are considered social species, meaning, in general, they prefer some form of group living. Species that live together must interact and so have evolved various behaviors that allow and facilitate group living. Environmental conditions and individual characteristics (e.g., sex, age, reproductive status, genetic background, etc…) are important in determining the form and amount of social interaction that occur within a group. In addition, sensory and motor abilities and health status can influence the expression of social behavior in individual animals. For example, an animal may be less willing to interact with others if it is ill or in pain. In another example, the sense of smell (olfaction) is extremely important in mouse communication and mice with olfactory deficiencies may behave quite differently than normal mice.

Before performing behavioral tests on rodents, especially when using unfamiliar strains or mutants, investigators must evaluate overall health and specific sensory and motor capabilities of the animals to avoid biased and inaccurate interpretations of the role of genetics in behavior.

Aggression and Social Dominance Behavior

Specific tests include the standard opponent test, isolation-induced fighting, resident-intruder test, and tube-test for social dominance. These tests are described below.

Aggressive behaviors are usually related to either territorial or maternal defensive actions or the establishment and maintenance of social status within a group. Males tend to show more territorial and social dominance behaviors than females but there are exceptions. Predatory behaviors (behavior oriented toward catching and killing of prey) are not included in this category. Rodents who bite humans are also not displaying true aggressive behavior but rather, fear induced defensive behavior.

Rats and mice differ in their social organization and use of aggressive behaviors. Male mice are territorial and do not tolerate unfamiliar males within their home range (or cage). Females may establish territories but tend not to defend them with aggressive behavior. Male (and female) mice mark territorial boundaries with urine; this is an important method of avoiding unnecessary aggression and its consequences in this species. In contrast, rats have evolved to live in multi-male/multi-female groups and tend to coexist peacefully if group composition is stable.

Although both mice and rats establish social dominance hierarchies within groups, they differ in important characteristics. Male social hierarchies in stable rat groups tend to stay the same despite changes in weight and/or size of individuals. In these types of groups, age may be the best predictor of social status. Male mice also establish social dominance hierarchies in a group but they will continuously compete for dominance. This often results in fighting and subsequent injuries. Changes in group composition, the presence of female mice in the room (olfactory stimulation) or manipulation of the mice (e.g., cage changing, temporary removal for experimental procedures) may increase fighting. If multiple mice are in the cage, removal of the dominant mouse will not necessarily stop the injuries, as the remaining mice will fight to reestablish a social order. Female mice and rats also establish social dominance hierarchies but tend not to fight. This makes it easier to group house them but harder to study social organization.

Standard Opponent Test

This test evaluates male aggression and social dominance in a test animal placed with an unfamiliar conspecific in a neutral area. The test subject is confined with a ‘standard opponent’ partner for a specific time in an unfamiliar cage or other defined space.

Important considerations for this test:

  1. The standard opponent(s) males are selected for highly replicable behavior as either submissive or dominant males in repeated tests with other males. Standard opponent partners are usually chosen from mouse strains known for either high or low levels of aggression. The selected mice are then used as standard opponent test partners in pairings with experimental mice.
  2. Differences in weight, age and size between the test mouse and the standard opponent may also influence test results.
  3. Keep in mind that it will be necessary to keep track of which mouse is the test subject and which is the ‘standard opponent’ during the test session. Mice with different coat colors will make this easier to do.
  4. Test sessions are often 5 minutes in length but are terminated early if attacks and biting are severe.
  5. The presence of humans can influence animal behavior during the test. The observer should be screened from the animals and/or the mice may be videotaped for scoring later.
  6. The frequencies of specific (predetermined) behaviors are scored. Examples of behaviors include body and anogenital sniffing sniffing, following and chasing, number and location of bites and tail rattling.

More information on standard opponent testing may be found in this and other references:

Crawley, JN. What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice, 2nd ed. Wiley-Interscience, 2007.

Isolation-Induced Fighting

This is a modification of the standard opponent test in which male mice are singly housed for a specific time period (e.g., four weeks) prior to placement with an unfamiliar male mouse into a test arena or cage.  Isolation of male mice tends to increase the frequency of fighting and attack behaviors.

Resident-Intruder Test

Another modification of the standard opponent test, the resident-intruder test is conducted in the home cage of the test mouse. The unfamiliar male mouse is the ‘intruder’. The test mouse (‘resident’) will attempt to defend its home cage from the intruder. Isolation is not needed prior to this test. Aggression in the resident mouse will be higher if he is living with a female and her litter (sired by him). However, the female and her pups must be separated from the fighting area, as the female will also display aggression toward the intruder (maternal defense).

Tube-Test for Social Dominance

This test measures dominant/submissive behavior in mice without allowing them to fight and injure each other. Both male and female mice may be tested with the Tube-Test. In the test, two mice of the same gender are placed at opposite ends of a clear, cylindrical tube and allowed to explore toward the tube center. At the point where the mice meet, the submissive mouse will tend to back up as the dominant mouse continues moving forward. The mouse that leaves the tube first (‘pushed out’) is the loser and the other mouse (dominant) is the winner. Automated equipment for this test exists that can measure additional parameters such as duration of match, latency to enter the tube, etc… This test can be used for determining social dominance relationships within a group of mice.

 Top of page

Rat and Mouse Parental and Maternal Behavior

Parental behaviors can be classified as direct (having an immediate physical impact on offspring and their survival) or indirect ((behaviors that do not involve physical contact but still affect offspring survival). Examples of direct behaviors include nursing, grooming or licking, retrieving and huddling. Some direct behaviors may be performed by males (i.e., the sire). Examples of indirect behaviors include nest building, defense against conspecifics or predators, acquiring and defending critical resources and care for pregnant or lactating females. Indirect behaviors may be performed by either parent and by other (non-parent) adults, which is referred to as alloparental care.

Although some laboratory studies indicate that adult male mice and rats are capable of parental behaviors, these occur at a low level and care of the young is primarily left to the female. Studies of wild mice and rats have shown that males are not involved in care of the young and will kill young that are not their own. Males will also kill unrelated or unfamiliar young under laboratory conditions. While the presence of the male sire in the breeding cage is generally not harmful to pups, there is no evidence that the male benefits pup growth and development. Adult males other than the sire, however, should not have access to young other than their own. See references 1 and 2 below for more information.

Maternal behavior typically refers to all aspects of behavior of the dam between parturition and weaning of the offspring and includes both direct and indirect behaviors. Some aspects of maternal behavior (e.g., nestbuilding) may begin prior to birth of the young. Laboratory studies with rodents have shown that hormonal changes (e.g., oxytocin) are important triggers for onset of maternal behavior. As hormonal influence decreases after parturition, infant stimulation increases in importance in this regard. Stimuli from pups, including ultrasonic vocalizations (USV), are needed to maintain maternal care after about 5 days postpartum (2, 3, 4). Infant rats and mice emit a variety of sonic and ultrasonic vocalizations that attract the dam’s attention. In mice, inbred strain differences in hearing ability and the number of USV emitted by pups have been found. USV have been extensively studied in rodents and various protocols for are available for experimental research (2,4).

In rats and mice, a postpartum estrus occurs within 24 hours after parturition. Laboratory studies have suggested that postpartum mating activities are shorter in duration than during normal estrus periods and do not significantly reduce maternal time spent with the litter (2). After the postpartum estrus period the female will not come into estrus again until after the pups are weaned. If she mated and conceived during the postpartum estrus, the second gestation may be prolonged by a week or more.

Young rats and mice are altricial, which means they are born in a relatively undeveloped state and cannot move, maintain body heat, see or hear on their own. Extensive maternal care is required for the young to survive. Rats and mice have evolved specific behaviors that contribute to the survival of altricial offspring. Both rats and mice will actively build nests in which to rear their young. These nests are built by the female and may be complex, multi-entrance enclosures if the dams are provided with appropriate building material. Significant strain differences in nest building skills have been shown in mice.

Both rats and mice will nest communally (multiple females rear their young in the same nest) and nurse offspring that are not their own. Laboratory studies have indicated that pup survival to weaning is higher for rats who rear their litters alone rather than in a communal nest. The opposite may be true in mice. Multiple studies have shown that mouse pups reared in communal nests had higher growth rates and better survival than pups reared alone with their dam (4). However, communal nesting/nursing may not be successful if the age difference between litters is greater than 5-7 days. In this situation, dams may be aggressive toward pups that are not their own.

Lactating females will display aggressive behavior to defend their offspring from others of their own species. The presence of pups appears to be the primary trigger for female postpartum aggression. The presence of unfamiliar male or female conspecifics will provoke maternal aggression although the likelihood and expression of maternal aggression varies with strain, individual and location (e.g., home cage versus test arena) (2,4).

There are a number of events and experiences that will influence the behavior of both the mother and the pups. These include the effects of handling of the dam and/or pups and disturbance of the cage environment by the researcher. Depending on the experimental objective these could be confounding factors and must be considered. Maternal behavior during lactation will also be affected by changes in the pups as they grow and mature and by the evolving physiological state of the dam.

Laboratory studies have shown that the main components of rodent maternal behavior (nursing, licking and grooming, pup retrieval and nest building) are present at high levels in almost all rats and mice after giving birth (2,4). Time spent in these behaviors typically declines gradually during the first two weeks of lactation and then decreases further or disappears during the third or fourth week after parturition. Consumption of food and water by the dam increases dramatically over the first two weeks of lactation and may influence the amount of time spent on maternal behaviors. Although commonly used as experimental measures of maternal behavior, nest building and pup retrieval do not normally occur at high frequencies in undisturbed conditions. Mice and rats build nests if material is available but once made, the nest is not rebuilt from scratch unless disturbed. Pup retrieval is also infrequently necessary under normal conditions.

Rat and mouse pups start eating solid food around 15-17 days of age and nursing by the dam ends by four weeks after gestation. Weaning of a litter is normally a gradual process that can stretch well beyond the third week. The typical abrupt weaning that takes place in the laboratory when the pups are 3-4 weeks of age provides another example of experimental manipulation influencing normal behavior.

References:

  1. Brown, R. E. (1986). Paternal behavior in the male long-evans rat (rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100(2), 162-172. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.100.2.162
  2. Elwood, R.W. (Ed.). (1983). Parental Behaviour of Rodents. Chichester: Wiley and Sons.
  3. Kazutaka, M., Nagasawa, M., Kikusui, T. (2011). Developmental consequences and biological significance of mother-infant bonding. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1232-1241.
  4. Weber, E.M. & Olsson, A.S. (2008). Maternal Behaviour in Mus musculus sp.: An ethological review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114, 1-22.

 

Stereoptypic Behavior in Rodents

Captive environments (e.g., cages) often reduce an animal’s ability to control and modify its environment, which can be stressful. Stress may lead to abnormal behavior and physiology. Abnormal repetitive behaviors (ARBs) are a type of abnormal behavior and are often referred to as stereotypies. ARBs are found in many species, including laboratory rodents, and are thought to occur as a result of brain malfunction. Therefore, once ARBs have developed, environmental enrichment may not eliminate them due to permanent developmental changes in brain physiology. There is evidence that the physiologic mechanisms (in the brain) that produce ARBs will also affect measures in behavioral experiments. This means that animals with ARBs may display other types of abnormal behavior that can influence results in behavioral testing. Even though environmental enrichment may not reverse established stereotypic behaviors, the provision of enrichment may prevent the development of abnormal behaviors such as ARBs, leading to improved validity, reliability, and replicability in behavioral experiments.

Reference: Garner JP. 2005. Stereotypies and Other Abnormal Repetitive Behaviors: Potential Impact on Validity, Reliability, and Replicability of Scientific Outcomes. ILAR Journal 46 (2): 106-117.